INFORMACIÓN

La revista Psicothema fue fundada en Asturias en 1989 y está editada conjuntamente por la Facultad y el Departamento de Psicología de la Universidad de Oviedo y el Colegio Oficial de Psicología del Principado de Asturias. Publica cuatro números al año.
Se admiten trabajos tanto de investigación básica como aplicada, pertenecientes a cualquier ámbito de la Psicología, que previamente a su publicación son evaluados anónimamente por revisores externos.

PSICOTHEMA
  • Director: Laura E. Gómez Sánchez
  • Periodicidad:
         Febrero | Mayo | Agosto | Noviembre
  • ISSN: 0214-9915
  • ISSN Electrónico: 1886-144X
CONTACTO
  • Dirección: Ildelfonso Sánchez del Río, 4, 1º B
    33001 Oviedo (España)
  • Teléfono: 985 285 778
  • Fax:985 281 374
  • Email: psicothema@cop.es

Psicothema, 1999. Vol. Vol. 11 (nº 2). 351-356




COHABITATION WITH A FEMALE INDUCES THE SAME LEVEL OF AGGRESSION AS ISOLATION IN OF1 MALE MICE

Luis Moya-Albiol, Àngels Calvo-Torrent, Yolanda Martí-Lianes, Alicia Salvador and Manuela Martínez

University of Valencia

The isolation or individually-housed paradigm is one of the most frequently used in male mice, although animals submitted to this procedure show alterations in some behavioral and physiological variables. In this study, the level of aggression induced by isolation was compared with that induced by cohabitation with a female in OF1 male mice. After a period of four weeks of either isolation or cohabitation with a female, male mice were confronted with a non-aggressive opponent in a neutral area for 10 minutes. Encounters were videotaped and the behavior of animals was analyzed. The results showed that both paradigms induced the same level of aggression in this strain. Furthermore, no differences in other behaviors were found. This study suggests that cohabitation with a female is not only a more natural but also a valid paradigm to induce aggression in OF1 male mice.

La cohabitación con una hembra induce el mismo nivel de agresión que el aislamiento en ratones macho OF1. Uno de los paradigmas más utilizados para inducir agresión en ratones machos es el aislamiento o alojamiento individual, a pesar de las alteraciones fisiológicas y conductuales que produce. En este estudio, se ha comparado el nivel de agresión inducido por aislamiento con el inducido mediante cohabitación con una hembra en ratones machos OF1. Tras cuatro semanas de aislamiento o cohabitación con una hembra, se confrontó a los ratones machos con un oponente no agresivo en una área neutral durante 10 minutos. Se registraron los encuentros y se analizó la conducta de los animales. Los resultados muestran que ambos paradigmas inducen el mismo nivel de agresión en esta cepa. Además, no se observaron diferencias en otras conductas. Este estudio sugiere que el paradigma de cohabitación con una hembra, siendo más natural que el aislamiento, es igualmente válido para inducir agresión en ratones machos OF1.

PDF

Different animal models are used in the laboratory to induce aggression in male rodents (Eichelman, Elliot and Barchas, 1981). The most commonly used in mice is the isolation paradigm, in which animals are individually housed for a period of some weeks before being confronted with another male. During this period, animals maintain visual, auditory and olfactory communication with other conspecifics in the same room. The level of aggressiveness increases with the duration of the isolation although it varies with strain and age (Valzelli and Garattini, 1968; Eleftheriou, Bailey and Denenberg, 1974; Eichelman et al., 1981; Siegfried, Alieva, Oliverio and Puglisi-Allegra, 1981; Puglisi-Allegra and Cabib, 1985).

It is well known that isolation has effects not only on aggression but also on other kinds of behavior, hormones and neurotransmitter systems (Valzelli, 1973; Valzelli and Bernasconi, 1979; Brain and Benton, 1983; Hilakivi, Ota and Lister, 1989; Cabib and Puglisi-Allegra, 1990). All these changes have been described as the «isolation syndrome» (Valzelli, 1973, 1981) which may possibly influence the effect of pharmacological treatments on aggression in pre-clinical studies.

In other rodent species such as rats the cohabitation with a female paradigm has been commonly used to induce aggression. However, the studies employing this paradigm in mice are scanty (Brain, Homady, Castaño and Parmigiani, 1987; Parmigiani, Mainardi, Brain, Haug and Brunoni, 1989; Yoshimura and Kimura, 1991). Few studies have compared the level of aggression induced by isolation and by cohabitation with a female (Brain, Benton and Bolton, 1978; O´Donnell, Blanchard and Blanchard, 1981; Jones and Brain, 1987; Zochi, Cabib and Puglisi-Allegra, 1994; Moya-Albiol, Salvador, Martínez-Sanchis and Costa, 1995) and the results obtained are different depending on the strain.

In order to study the possibility of using a more natural paradigm to induce aggression than the «isolation» model, the present study compared the level of aggression induced by isolation which that induced by cohabitation with a female in OF1 male mice.

Materials and methods

Subjects and Housing

Ninety nine commercially-acquired (IFFA CREDO, Lyon, France) OF1 albino mice (86 males and 13 females) arrived at the laboratory at 42 days of age. One group of males (n = 60) was housed in groups of five in opaque plastic cages measuring 24.5 x 24.5 x 15 cm (PANLAB, Barcelona, Spain) and used as non-aggressive anosmic opponents. They were rendered anosmic 24 hours before agonistic encounters. Each mouse was lightly anaesthetized with ether and then 25 ul of a 4% aqueous solution of zinc sulphate was introduced into each nostril. They were inverted shortly afterwards to prevent their swallowing the toxic solution (Alberts and Galef, 1971; Brain, Goldsmith, Parmigiani and Mainardi, 1982). Another group (n = 26) was used as experimental animals. Half of them were individually housed in transparent plastic cages measuring 24 x 14 x 13 cm (LETICA S.A. Barcelona, Spain) and the other half were paired with a female in an identical cage. All animals lived under a reversed lighting schedule (white lights on from 20:00 to 08:00 hr local time) and were maintained at 18-21ºC. Food and water were supplied ad libitum.

Agonistic encounters

Four weeks after living in isolation or cohabiting with a female each experimental male was submitted to an agonistic encounter with an anosmic opponent in an all-glass neutral cage (60 x 33 x 30 cm) located in an observation room. They were initially separated by a plastic partition for an adaptation period of one minute and, after removal of the partition, they interacted for ten minutes. All encounters were carried out during the dark period (between the third and sixth hour of the dark phase), under a red light from a 25 watt bulb. The sawdust substrate was changed and the cage cleaned after each encounter.

Behavioral analysis

Encounters were video-taped using a Panasonic NV camera, a Panasonic NV-770 video and a Sony Trinitron monitor. The behavioral analysis involved assessment of the behavior of the experimental male during the agonistic encounter. Tapes were analyzed using an ethologically-based methodology assisted by a microprocessor (Brain, McAllister and Walmsley, 1989). The computer program enabled the assessment of total duration, frequency and latency of eleven broad categories of behavior. Each category consisted of groups of well-defined specific acts, postures or elements of behavior (Martínez, Castaño, Simón and Brain, 1986) that are given in Table I.

Statistical Analysis

Data of the total duration, frequency and latency of each broad behavioral category exhibited by isolated or cohabiting with a female male mice were subjected to appropriate paired comparisons using Mann-Whitney «U» test. A probability level less than 0.05 was considered as significant.

Results

The results of the behavior shown by isolated or cohabiting with a female mice are given in Table II. No defense, avoidance, immobility or sexual behaviors were recorded in either group. As can be seen in Table II no significant differences were found between groups in any of the behavioral categories recorded.

Discussion

The results indicate that the same level of intermale aggression (attack and threat) is induced either by isolation or cohabitation with a female although the level of aggression was low and the latency to first aggression long in comparison with previous studies with the isolation paradigm (Martínez, Salvador and Simón, 1994). Our results agree with other studies which found that male mice cohabiting with a female showed a similar level of intermale aggression to isolated ones, although higher or lower levels of aggression have also been reported in males cohabiting with a female. It has been suggested that one of the reasons for the different results obtained could be the genetic differences. In this sense, similar levels of aggression have been reported in TO and Swiss albino outbred strains (O´Donnell et al., 1981; Brain and Benton, 1983; Jones and Brain, 1987) with both paradigms, while a higher level of aggression induced by isolation in DBA/2 and C57BL/10 strains (Jones and Brain, 1987; Zochi et al., 1994), and a higher level of aggression induced by cohabitation in TO and C57BL/6 strains (Brain et al., 1978; Zochi et al., 1994) have also been reported. Thus, it seems that susceptibility to the isolation and the cohabiting-induced aggression differs between strains. In our study we have used male mice of the OF1 outbred strain and the results show that this strain is susceptible to a display of aggression after both paradigms.

On the other hand, no significant differences were found in any of the other five behavioral categories recorded. Only a few studies have also paid attention to other behaviors displayed by the animals during the agonistic encounter. To this respect our results differ from those carried out on TO male mice in which males cohabitating with a female spent less time in social exploration and more time in non social exploration than isolated mice (Brain et al., 1978). As seen in the results, no differences were found between the two groups in our study in either of those behaviors.

Two important questions arise from these studies: 1) what are the mechanisms underlying the induction of aggression by these two paradigms? and 2) what are the genetic differences involved in the differences between strains? In addition, it is important to bear in mind that the changes produced by these two living conditions might influence the results of the pharmacological studies carried out to assess the biological bases of aggression.

In conclusion, this study shows that isolation and cohabitation with a female induce the same level of aggression in OF1 male mice. Considering that cohabitation with a female is more natural than the isolation paradigm, we should think about the possibility of using the former paradigm in studies on intermale aggression in this outbred strain of mice.

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank Miriam Phillips for the revision of the English text and Ferran Dual for animal care.

 

Alberts, J.R. and Galef, B.G. Jr. (1971). Acute anosmia in the rat: a behavioral test of peripherally-induced olfactory deficit. Physiology and Behavior, 6, 619-621.

Brain, P.F. and Benton, D. (1983). Conditions of housing, hormones, and aggressive behavior. In B. Svare (ed.), Hormones and Aggressive Behavior (pp 351-372) New York: Plenum Press.

Brain, P.F., Benton, D. and Bolton, J.C. (1978). Comparison of agonistic behavior in individually-housed male mice with those cohabiting with females. Aggressive Behavior, 4, 201-206.

Brain, P.F., Goldsmith, J.F., Parmingiani, S. and Mainardi, M. (1982). Involvement of various senses in responses to individual housing in laboratory albino mice. 1. The olfactory sense. Bolletino di Zoologia, 49, 213-222.

Brain, P.F., Homady, M.H., Castaño, D. and Parmigiani, S. (1987). «Pheromones» and behaviour of rodents and primates. Bolletino di Zoologia, 4, 279-288.

Brain, P.F., McAllister, K.H. and Walmsley, S. (1989). Drug effects on social behaviour: Methods in ethopharmacology. In A.A. Boulton, G.B. Baker, A.J. Greenshaw (eds.), Neuromethods (pp. 687-739). The Humana Press Inc.

Cabib, S. and Puglisi-Allegra, S. (1990). Social behavior of the house mouse: a potential model for preclinical studies on stress. In S. Puglisi-Allegra, A. Oliverio (eds.), Psychobiology of stress (pp 31-40). The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

Eichelman, B., Elliott, G.R. and Barchas, J.D. (1981). Biochemical, Pharmacological and Genetic Aspects of Aggression. In A.R. Liss (ed.), Biobehavioral Aspects of Aggression (pp 51-84). New York.

Eleftheriou, B.E., Bailey, D.W. and Denenberg, V.H. (1974). Genetic Analysis of fighting behavior in mice. Physiology and Behavior, 15, 731-736.

Hilakivi, L.A., Ota, M. and Lister, R.G. (1989). Effect of isolation on brain monoamines and the behavior of mice in tests of exploration, locomotion, anxiety and behavioral ‘despair’. Pharmacology, Biochemistry & Behavior, 33, 371-374.

Jones, S.E. and Brain, P.F. (1987). Performances of inbred and outbred laboratory mice in putative tests of aggression. Behavior Genetics, 17, 87-96.

Martínez, M., Salvador, A. and Simón, V.M. (1994). Behavioral changes over several successful agonistic encounters between male mice: effects of type of «standard opponent». Aggressive Behavior, 20, 441-451.

Martínez, M., Castaño, D., Simón, V. and Brain, P.F. (1986). An ethopharmacological assessment of the influences of ciproterone acetate on social interactions in male mice. IRCS Medical Science, 14, 44-45.

Moya-Albiol, L., Salvador, A., Martínez-Sanchis, S. and Costa, R. (1995). Efectos de la administración aguda de propionato de testosterona en dos modelos de agresión en laboratorio. Revista de Psicologia Universitas Tarraconensis, 17(2), 117-144.

O’Donnell, V., Blanchard, R.J. and Blanchard, D.C. (1981). Mouse aggression increases after 24 hours of isolation or housing with female. Behavioral and Neural Biology, 32, 89-103.

Parmigiani, S., Mainardi, M., Brain, P.F., Haug, M. and Brunoni, V. (1989). Variation in aggressive behavior and anatomo-physiological correlates generated by crowding without physical contact in the house mouse. Aggressive Behavior, 15, 191-200.

Puglisi-Allegra, S. and Cabib, S. (1985). The effect of age on two kinds of aggressive behavior in inbred strains of mice. Developmental Psychobiology, 18(6), 477-482.

Siegfried, B., Alleva, S., Oliverio, A. and Puglisi-Allegra, S. (1981). Effects of isolation on activity, reactivity, excitability and aggressive behavior in two inbred strains of mice. Behavioral Brain Research, 2, 211-218.

Valzelli, L. (1973). The isolation syndrome in mice. Psycopharmacologia, 31, 305-320.

Valzelli, L. (1981). Psychobiology of aggression and violence. New York: Raven Press.

Valzelli, L. and Bernasconi, S. (1979). Aggressiveness by isolation and brain serotonin turnover changes in different strains of mice. Neuropsychobiology, 5, 129-135.

Valzelli, L. and Garattini, S. (1968). Behavioral changes and 5-hydroxytryptamine turnover in animals. In S. Garattini, P.A. Shore (eds.), Proceedings of the International Congress on Biological Role of Indolealkylamine Derivates (pp 249). New York: Academic Press Inc.

Yoshimura, H. and Kimura, N. (1991). Ethopharmacology of copulatory disorder induced by chronic social conflict in male mice. Neuroscience Biobehavioral Review, 15, 497-500.

Zochi, A., Cabib, S. and Puglisi-Allegra, S. (1994). Opposite strain-dependent differences for intermale aggressive behavior elicited by individual housing and housing with a female in the mouse. Aggressive Behavior, 20, 305-314.

Aceptado el 27 de julio de 1998

Impact Factor JCR SSCI Clarivate 2023 = 3.2 (Q1) / CiteScore SCOPUS 2023 = 6.5 (Q1)